Phonological Awareness Literature Review Chapter

Total Length: 2667 words ( 9 double-spaced pages)

Total Sources: 8

Page 1 of 9

Phonological Awareness and Literacy

It appears that in the last ten years that there has been a growing consensus on the range of skills that have been serving as the basis for reading and writing ability in the 3- to 5-year-old age group (Diamond K., 2014). In order to become a skilled reader before kindergarten, children need a language that is rich and they need something with conceptual knowledge base, and to be able to understand messages that are communicated through print. It is also important that children also must be able to develop the notion that spoken words are made up of smaller substances of speech (phonological awareness) before they enter kindergarten. (Gallagher, 2015)

Nonetheless to attain a high level of skill, the 3- to 5-year-old group need chances to change these strands, not in isolation, but then again interactively. Making the point, not sounds or letters, encourages the 3- to 5-year-old group earliest experiences when it comes to print (Noe, 2014). Given the tremendous amount of attention that early literacy has been getting lately in policy arenas (Miller, 2014), and the rising diversity of the child population, it is significant and appropriate to take stock of these serious dimensions in addition to the strong points and gaps in their ability to calculate these skills successfully.

Analysis

Research shows that verbal abilities among the 3-5 age group are reliably the best forecasters of later reading accomplishment before coming to kindergarten (Stacey L. Tucci, 2015). Those that are skilled readers normally put together multiple levels of the language system (Gallagher, 2015), with abilities surrounding grammar, terminology, and discourse. Also, further investigation displays that vocabulary size matters. This is because optimal settings could increase exponentially in the years that were much earlier (Rutherford, 2012), with the 3- to 5-year-old group learning to recognize words uttered to them before they can even start saying them on their own. Research shows that word knowledge, thus far, is not the one that is considered to be the most established, but to knowledge-building language experiences (Neuman, 2001) that involve the 3- to 5-year-old group in developing and purifying networks of definitely-related ideas.

When these children are able to have the right kind of practice, the 3- to 5-year-old word knowledge is put to use in different kinds of structures. 3- to 5-year-old sentences usually start at around two words (Miller, 2014), then rapidly raise to four or more words as children interrelate their thoughts gradually by plans of language. Other experts (Noe, 2014) have shown that conversations that are tangibly take off from immediate events or projects ('let's say?') are tied to the development of abstract reasoning and related to literacy skills like print production and narrative competence.

With word learning happening so rapidly, 3- to 5-year-old start to make a lot of distinctions of words that are clear and make good sense before coming to kindergarten. These distinctions are not just founded on their meaning but also founded on the way they sound. Furthermore, these differences start making comparisons that are hidden among words that are alike in the way they sound, a marvel 3- to 5-year-old explained by linguists as lexical restructuring (Rutherford, 2012). Let's say, a four-year-old child perhaps recognizes the words "mut" from "mat;" "lot" from "cot." Differentiating between these parallel sounding words both accurately and quickly, 3- to 5-year-old start to hear sequences of sound that establish each recognized word. Furthermore, the 3- to 5-year-olds that are able to articulate large words turn out to be agreed to these sections and obtain new words quickly; 3- to 5-year-old with smaller words may be restricted to more global differences. As a result, vocabulary size and vocabulary percentage are significant for lexical rearrangement (Gallagher, 2015), and are strongly tied to the development of phonological consciousness.

Recent analyses (Miller, 2014) have made it abundantly obvious, on the other hand, that oral language, and more specifically vocabulary development, not only play a role in phonological awareness nevertheless likewise are critical skills for the expansion of reading understanding later on. As a result, it is vital for quality pointers in early childhood programs to be able to recognize that vocabulary development and oral language is the foundation for all other skills serious to reading that is successful especially before coming to kindergarten.

Founded to help rising young readers (Stacey L. Tucci, 2015), phonological awareness is looked at as being a critical precursor to help 3- to 5-year-olds read and pronounce words better.

Stuck Writing Your "Phonological Awareness" Literature Review Chapter?

Various units of language (phonemes, segments, words,) is strongly linked to successful reading (Miller, 2014). It is, however, as defined above, both a reason and a significance of learning to read and vocabulary development (Baroody, 2015). Normally developing 3- to 5-year-old kids start to distinguish among units of language (phonological awareness), then inside these units (phonemic awareness). According to Gallagher (2015) phonological awareness denotes to the overall capacity to attend to the sounds of language as different from its meaning. It is also means that phonemic awareness is the understanding that every verbal word can be regarded as units of sounds that are signified by the letter of the ABCs (Stacey L. Tucci, 2015).

According to Diamond K (2014) they bring eevidence that proposes that children attain syllabic understanding previously than they attain understanding to phonemes, and sensitivity to assonance before understanding to phonemes. Kids in the 3- to 5-year-old group have entry to these skills and they typically start with linguistic activities for instance language games and nursery rhymes (Noe, 2014) that indirectly compare and contrast the sounds of words, and consist of repetitive expressions such as (zibbily zobbily zoo begins with / z/). Then again understood comparisons, alone, may be not enough. Research shows that phonemic awareness and phonological awareness are meta-linguistic skills (Baroody, 2015). Those in the 3- to 5-year-old bracket must not only be able to play and narrate with sound units, they must likewise understand the awareness that sound makes as part of written language.

According to Baroody (2015) pphonological awareness should not be misperceived with phonics. The term decoding or phonics undertakes that 3- to 5-year-olds are able to grab the meaning that the phonemic structure of words, and also the (sound/letter) relationship. Research that have tried make learning easy through primary phonics training have shown no effects (Gallagher, 2015); actually, evidence proposes that such training, that does not have a complete understanding of phonemic awareness, may be detrimental to learning to spell and remembering words.

Recent analyses and reviews (Diamond K., 2014) have put phonological awareness as a significant part of a difficult braid of language skills which consist of strands of phonology, pragmatics, semantics, syntax, and dialogue. Its tie to 3- to 5-year-old age group ability to decipher has been clearly recognized. Together, quality pointers would do well to identify that phonological awareness skills are naturally linked to other important language skills which need to be powerfully reinforced in these early care and education programs.

Knowledge of the writing system letters is a strong forecaster of short- and continuing reading achievement (Miller, 2014). Nevertheless, its effect on later reading is not about recognizing the letter names, as such. Somewhat, the learning of letter names act as a go-between the skill to be able to recollect the sounds that are linked with the letters (Noe, 2014). Once more, there is a relationship that is reciprocal among skills: knowledge of letter plays a powerful role in the advancement of phonological awareness, and advanced levels of understanding the letter are all connected with 3- to 5-year-old group abilities to figure out and control phonemes. For instance, the child who is able to recognize the letter 'c' is to be expected to remember the sound of / k/. As a result, letter knowledge can sometimes reflect a bigger underlying knowledge and expertise with literacy associated skills for example print and language.

According to Gallagher (2015) research shows that 3- to 5-year-olds distinguish letters consistent with their visual form, that is to say, their horizontal, upright and diagonal segments. Assuming that the difficulties of the visually distinct forms of letters (lower case, upper case, printed form), present learning theory (Diamond K., 2014) proposes that all together teaching two versions of uppercase and lowercase letters with their confusable sounds and labels can be overpowering to the young child. Yet, there is no significant evidence to recommend which particular form (lower or upper case) should be communicated initially.

There is thorough research that shows an increasing body of research making the proposal that there is a diversity of extrinsic and intrinsic issues that are influencing the formation of letter knowledge. Further research suggests that exposure to letters is a looked at as being a primary vehicle for writing system knowledge. Those that are 3- to 5-year-old and who participate regularly in adult-child writing activities that consist of a conscious focus on print have better alphabet.....

Show More ⇣


     Open the full completed essay and source list


OR

     Order a one-of-a-kind custom essay on this topic


Related Essays

The Various Ways to Learn

programs that promote early literacy through oral language, phonological awareness, print and word awareness, and alphabet knowledge prepare children to be more successful in kindergarten and more effective readers" (Knoche & Davis, 2016, p. 181). People… Continue Reading...

sample essay writing service

Cite This Resource:

Latest APA Format (6th edition)

Copy Reference
"Phonological Awareness" (2015, March 26) Retrieved May 6, 2024, from
https://www.aceyourpaper.com/essays/phonological-awareness-2149338

Latest MLA Format (8th edition)

Copy Reference
"Phonological Awareness" 26 March 2015. Web.6 May. 2024. <
https://www.aceyourpaper.com/essays/phonological-awareness-2149338>

Latest Chicago Format (16th edition)

Copy Reference
"Phonological Awareness", 26 March 2015, Accessed.6 May. 2024,
https://www.aceyourpaper.com/essays/phonological-awareness-2149338